References

Cognitive Psychology: Theory, Process, and Methodology - Dawn M. McBride, J. Cooper Cutting 2019


References

Abrams, R. A., & Balota, D. A. (1991). Mental chronometry: Beyond reaction time. Psychological Science, 2, 153—157.

Abu-Obeid, N. (1998). Abstract and scenographic imagery: The effect of environmental form on wayfinding. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 18, 159—173.

Aitchison, J. (2003). Words in the mind: An introduction to the mental lexicon (3rd ed.). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Allen, S. W., & Brooks, L. R. (1991). Specializing the operation of an explicit rule. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 3—19.

Altmann, G. T. M. (1998). Ambiguity in sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 146—152.

Amit, E., & Greene, J. D. (2012). You see, the ends don’t justify the means: Visual imagery and moral judgment. Psychological Science, 23, 861—868.

Anderson, J. R. (1976). Language, memory, and associative memory. Washington, DC: Hemisphere Press.

Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (1973). Human associative memory. Washington, DC: Winson.

Anglin, J. M. (1977). Word, object and conceptual development. New York: Norton.

Arnold, J. E., Eisenband, J. G., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Trueswell, J. C. (2000). The rapid use of gender information: Evidence of the time course of pronoun resolution from eye tracking. Cognition, 76, B13—B26.

Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 89—195). New York: Academic Press.

Baars, B. J. (2007). Attention and consciousness. In B. J. Baars & N. M. Gage (Eds.), Cognition, brain, and consciousness: Introduction to cognitive neuroscience. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556—559.

Baddeley, A. D. (1998). Recent developments in working memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 234—238.

Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417—423.

Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1—29.

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47—89). New York: Academic Press.

Baddeley, A. D., Hitch, G. J., & Allen, R. J. (2009). Working memory and binding in sentence recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 438—456.

Baddeley, A. D., Lewis, V., & Vallar, G. (1984). Exploring the articulatory loop. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A, 233—252.

Baddeley, A. D., Thompson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575—589.

Bahrick, H. P. (1984). Semantic memory content in permastore: Fifty years of memory for Spanish learned in school. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 1—29.

Balota, D. A. (1990). The role of meaning in word processing. In D. A. Balota, G. Flores D’Arcais, & K. Rayner (Eds.), Comprehension processes in reading (pp. 9—32). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Balota, D. A., & Chumbley, J. I. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 340—357.

Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (1994). Implicit stereotyping in judgements of fame. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 181—198.

Barron, E., Riby, L. M., Greer, J., & Smallwood, J. (2011). Absorbed in thought: The effect of mind wandering on the processing of relevant and irrelevant events. Psychological Science, 22, 596—601.

Barsalou, L. W. (1985). Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 629—654.

Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577—660.

Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617—645.

Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Grounded cognition: Past, present, and future. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 716—724.

Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Batteli, L., Pascual-Leone, A., & Cavanagh, P. (2007). The “when” pathway of the right parietal lobe. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 204—210.

Beall, A. C., & Loomis, J. M. (1997) Optic flow and visual analysis of the base-to-final turn. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 7, 201—223.

Belleville, S., Clément, F., Mellah, S., Gilbert, B., Fontaine, F., & Gauthier, S. (2011). Training-related brain plasticity in subjects at risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 134, 1623—1634.

Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2012). At 6—9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 3253—3258.

Berlin, B. (1992). Ethnobiological classification. Principles of categorization of plants and animals in traditional societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94, 115—147.

Bilalic, M., McLeod, P., & Gobet, F. (2008). Why good thoughts block better ones: The mechanism of the pernicious Einstellung (set) effect. Cognition, 108, 652—661.

Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (1992). A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation. In A. Healy, S. Kosslyn, & R. Shiffrin (Eds.), From learning processes to cognitive processes: Essays in honor of William K. Estes (Vol. 2, pp. 35—67). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Bjork, R. A., & Whitten, W. B. (1974). Recency-sensitive retrieval processes in long-term free recall. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 173—189.

Blaxton, T. A. (1989). Investigating dissociations among memory measures: Support for a transfer appropriate processing framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 657—668.

Bock, J. K. (1996). Language production: Methods and methodologies. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 34, 395—421.

Bock, J. K., & Eberhard, K. M. (1993). Meaning, sound, and syntax in English number agreement. Language & Cognitive Processes, 8, 57—99.

Bock, J. K., & Miller, C. A. (1991). Broken agreement. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 45—93.

Bock, K., Eberhard, K., Cutting, J., Meyer, A., & Schriefers, H. (2001). Some attractions of verb agreement. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 83—128.

Böckler, A., van der Wel, P. R. D., & Welsh, T. N. (2014). Catching eyes: Effects of social and nonsocial cues on attention capture. Psychological Science, 25, 720—727.

Borchers, S., Christensen, A., Ziegler, L., & Himelbach, M. (2010). Visual action control does not rely on strangers: Effects of pictorial cues under monocular and binocular vision. Neuropsychologia, 49, 556—563.

Bowden, E. M., Jung-Beeman, M., Fleck, J., & Kounios, J. (2005). New approaches to demystifying insight. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 322—328.

Braine, M. D. S. (1978). On the relation between the natural logic of reasoning and standard logic. Psychological Review, 85, 1—21.

Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (1998). Fuzzy-trace theory and children’s false memories. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 81—129.

Brandone, A. C., Pence, K. L., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2007). Action speaks louder than words: Young children differentially weight perceptual, social, and linguistic cues to learn verbs. Child Development, 78, 1322—1342.

Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., & Cleland, A. A. (1999). Syntactic priming in written production: Evidence for rapid decay. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 635—640.

Bransford, J. D., & Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 717—726.

Bransford, J. D., & Stein, B. S. (1993). The ideal problem solver: A guide for improving thinking, learning, and creativity (2nd ed.). New York: W. H. Freeman.

Brewer, M. B., Dull, V., & Lui, L. (1981). Perceptions of the elderly: Stereotypes as prototypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 656—670.

Brewer, W. F., & Treyens, J. C. (1981). Role of schemata in memory for places. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 207—230.

Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. London: Pergamon Press.

Broca, P. (1861). Remarques sur le siége de la faculté. de la parole articulé, suivies d’une observation d’aphémie (perte de parole). Bulletin de la Société d’Anatomie (Paris), 36, 330—357.

Brooks, L. R. (1968). Spatial and verbal components of the act of recall. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 22, 349—368.

Brown, J. (1958). Some tests of the decay theory of immediate memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10, 12—21.

Brown, R. (1958). Words and things. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.

Bullock, T. H. (1961). Four notes for discussion. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Byrne, R. M. J. (2002). Mental models and counterfactual thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 405—445.

Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1243—1249.

Campo, N. S., Gregory, A. H., & Fisher, R. (2012). Interviewing behaviors in police investigators: A field study of a current U.S. sample. Psychology, Crime, and Law, 18, 359—375.

Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Castel, A. D., McCabe, D. P., Roediger, H. L., III, & Heitman, J. L. (2007). The dark side of expertise: Domain-specific memory errors. Psychological Science, 18, 3—5.

Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). The mind’s eye in chess. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 215—281). New York: Academic Press.

Chater, N., & Oaksford, M. R. (1999). The probability heuristics model of syllogistic reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 191—258.

Chein, J. M., & Weisberg, R. W. (2014). Working memory and insight in verbal problems: Analysis of compound and remote associates. Memory & Cognition, 42, 67—83.

Cheng, P. W., & Holyoak, K. J. (1985). Pragmatic reasoning schemas. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 391—416.

Cheng, P. W., & Novick, L. R. (1992). Covariation in natural causal induction. Psychological Review, 99, 365—382.

Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975—979.

Chi, M. T. H. (2006). Two approaches to the study of experts’ characteristics. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 21—30). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121—152.

Chi, R. P., & Snyder, A. W. (2011). Facilitate insight by non-invasive brain stimulation. PLoS One, 6, 181—197.

Chi, R. P., & Snyder, A. W. (2012). Brain stimulation enables the solution of an inherently difficult problem. Neuroscience Letters, 515, 121—124.

Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton.

Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Language, 35, 26—58.

Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Chronicle, E. P., Ormerod, T. C., & MacGregor, J. N. (2001). When insight just won’t come: The failure of visual cues in the nine-dot problem. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 54(A), 903—919.

Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Clifasefi, S. L., Bernstein, D. M., Mantonakis, A., & Loftus, E. F. (2013). “Queasy does it”: False alcohol beliefs and memories may lead to diminished alcohol preferences. Acta Psychologica, 143, 14—19.

Coane, J. H., & McBride, D. M. (2006). The role of test structure in creating false memories. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1026—1036.

Cohen, B., & Murphy, G. L. (1984). Models of concepts. Cognitive Science, 8, 27—58.

Cohen, G., & Faulkner, D. (1989). Age differences in source forgetting: Effects on reality monitoring and eyewitness testimony. Psychology and Aging, 4, 10—17.

Colbert, J. M., & McBride, D. M. (2007). Comparing decay rates for accurate and false memories in the DRM paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1600—1609.

Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407—428.

Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 241—248.

Conrad, R. (1964). Acoustic confusion in immediate memory. British Journal of Psychology, 55, 75—84.

Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., & Bunting, M. F. (2001). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 331—335.

Corballis, M. C. (2010). Mirror neurons and the evolution of language. Brain and Language, 112, 25—35.

Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31, 187—316.

Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 163—191.

Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-processes model of working memory. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 62—101). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87—185.

Cox, C. S., Chee, E., Chase, G. A., Baumgardner, T. L., Schuerholz, L. J., Reader, M. J., et al. (1997). Reading proficiency affects the construct validity of the Stroop Test Interference Score. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 11, 105—110.

Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268—294.

Cree, G. S., McRae, K., & McNorgan, C. (1999). An attractor model of lexical conceptual processing: Simulating semantic priming. Cognitive Science, 23, 371—414.

Cruse, D. A. (1977). The pragmatics of lexical specificity. Journal of Linguistics, 11, 153—164.

Crutcher, R. J., & Beer, J. M. (2011). An auditory analog of the picture superiority effect. Memory & Cognition, 39, 63—74.

Cummins, D. D. (1995). Naïve theories and causal deduction. Memory & Cognition, 23, 646—658.

Cutting, C. J., & Ferreira, V. S. (1999). Semantic and phonological information flow in the production lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 318—344.

Damian, M. F., & Martin, R. C. (1999). Semantic and phonological factors interact in single word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 345—361.

Darwin, C. T., Turvey, M. T., & Crowder, R. G. (1974). An auditory analogue of the Sperling partial report procedure: Evidence for brief auditory storage. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 255—267.

Davidson, J. E. (1995). The suddenness of insight. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 125—155). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Davidson, J. E., & Sternberg, R. J. (1986). What is insight? Educational Horizons, 64, 177—179.

DeCasper, A. J., Lecanuet, J.-P., Busnel, M. C., Granier-Deferre, C., & Maugeais, R. (1994). Fetal reactions to recurrent maternal speech. Infant Behaviour and Development, 17(2), 159—164.

Decety, J., & Grèzes, J. (2006). The power of simulation: Imagining one’s own and other’s behavior. Brain Research, 24, 4—14.

Deese, J. (1950). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17—22.

de Groot, A. D. (1966). Perception and memory versus thought: Some old ideas and recent findings. In B. Kleinmuntz (Ed.), Problem solving: Research, method and theory (pp. 19—50). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283—321.

Del Pinal, G., & Nathan, M. J. (2013). There and up again: On the uses and misuses of neuroimaging in psychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 30, 233—252.

Demers, R. A. (1988). Linguistics and animal communication. In F. J. Newmeyer (Ed.), Linguistics: The Cambridge survey (pp. 314—335). New York: Cambridge University Press.

De Neys, W. (2006). Dual processing in reasoning: Two systems but one reasoner. Psychological Science, 17(5), 428—433.

De Neys, W., Vartanian, O., & Goel, V. (2008). Smarter than we think: When our brains detect that we are biased. Psychological Science, 19, 483—489.

Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5—18.

Dewey, J. (1910). How we think. Boston, MA: Heath.

Dickstein, L. S. (1981). Conversion and possibility in syllogistic reasoning. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 18, 229—232.

Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). Think different: The merits of unconscious thought in preference development and decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 586—598.

Dijksterhuis, A., & Nordgren, L. F. (2006). A theory of unconscious thought. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 95—109.

Dominey, P. F., & Dodane, C. (2004). Indeterminacy in language acquisition: The role of child directed speech and joint attention. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 121—145.

Donders, F. C. (1969). On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica, 30, 412—431. [Translation of Die Schnelligkeit psychischer Processe, first published in 1868.]

Dronkers, N. F., Plaisant, O., Iba-Zizen, M. T., & Cabanis, E. A. (2007). Paul Broca’s historic cases: High resolution MR imaging of the brains of Leborgne and Lelong. Brain, 130, 1432—1441.

Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58(5), i—113.

Düzel, E., Yonelinas, A. P., Mangun, G. R., Heinz, H.-J., & Tulving, E. (1997). Event-related brain potential correlates of two states of conscious awareness in memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94, 5973—5978.

Eagle, M., & Leiter, E. (1964). Recall and recognition in intentional and incidental learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 58—63.

Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. Translated by H. A. Ruger & C. E. Bussenius (1913). New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.

Eich, E. (1995). Searching for mood dependent memory. Psychological Science, 6, 67—75.

Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P., & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303—306.

Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 717—726.

Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Thomas, R., Mayfield, S., Shank, H., Morrisette, N., et al. (2005). Multiple processes in prospective memory retrieval: Factors determining monitoring versus spontaneous retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 327—342.

Ellis, N. C., & Hennelly, R. A. (1980). A bilingual word-length effect: Implications for intelligence testing and the relative ease of mental calculation in Welsh and English. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 43—51.

Emberson, L. L., Lupyan, G., Goldstein, M. H., & Spivey, M. J. (2010). Overheard cell-phone conversations: When less speech is more distracting. Psychological Science, 21, 1383—1388.

Epstein, M. L. (1980). The relationship of mental imagery and mental rehearsal to performance of a motor task. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 2, 211—220.

Erard, M. (2007). Um...: Slips, stumbles, and verbal blunders, and what they mean. New York: Random House.

Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L., et al. (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 3017—3022.

Evans, J. St. B. T. (1984). Heuristic and analytic processes in reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 451—468.

Evans, J. St. B. T. (2006). The heuristic-analytic theory of reasoning: Extension and evaluation. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 378—395.

Evans, J. St. B. T. (2008). Dual-process accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255—278.

Evans, J. St. B. T. (2012). Questions and challenges to the new psychology of reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 18(1), 5—31.

Fajen, B., Riley, M., & Turvey, M. (2009). Information, affordances, and the control of action in sport. Journal of Sport Psychology, 40(1), 79—107.

Feeney, A., & Heit, E. (2007). Inductive reasoning: Experimental, developmental and computational approaches. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Fernandez-Duque, D., & Johnson, M. L. (1999). Attention metaphors: How metaphors guide the cognitive psychology of attention. Cognitive Science, 23, 83—116.

Fiebelkorn, I. C., Foxe, J. J., Schwartz, T. H., & Molholm, S. (2010). Staying within the lines: The formation of visuospatial boundaries influences multisensory feature integration. European Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1737—1743.

Foer, J. (2011). Moonwalking with Einstein: The art and science of remembering everything. New York: Penguin Press.

Foley, J. E., & Cohen, A. J. (1984). Mental mapping of megastructure. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 38, 440—453.

Ford, M. (1994). Two modes of representation and problem solution in syllogistic reasoning. Cognition, 54, 1—71.

Fouts, R. S., Fouts, D. H., & Van Canfort, T. E. (1989). The infant Loulis learns signs from cross-fostered chimpanzees. In R. A. Gardner, B. T. Gardner, & T. E. Van Cantfort (Eds.), Teaching sign language to chimpanzees. Albany: State University of New York Press.

Fox Tree, J. E. (2001). Listeners’ uses of um and uh in speech comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 29(2), 320—326.

Francis, W. S. (2005). Bilingual semantic and conceptual representation. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 251—267). New York: Oxford University Press.

Frazier, L. (1987). Sentence processing: A tutorial review. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance: Vol. XII. The psychology of reading (pp. 559—586). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Frazier, L., & Fodor, J. D. (1978). The sausage machine: A new two-stage parsing model. Cognition, 6, 291—325.

Fromkin, V. A. (1971). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language, 47, 27—52.

Fugelsang, J. A., Thompson, V. A., & Dunbar, K. N. (2006). Examining the representation of causal knowledge. Thinking & Reasoning, 12, 1—30.

Gallo, D. A. (2010). False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the DRM illusion. Memory & Cognition, 38, 833—848.

Galotti, K. M. (1989). Approaches to studying formal and everyday reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 331—351.

Galotti, K. M. (2002). Making decisions that matter: How people face important life choices. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Ganel, T., Tanzer, M., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). A double dissociation between action and perception in the context of visual illusions: Opposite effects of real and illusory size. Psychological Science, 19, 221—225.

Gardner, R. A., & Gardner, B. T. (1969). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science, 165, 664—672.

Garrett, M. F. (1975). The analysis of sentence production. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 9). New York: Academic Press.

Garrett, M. F. (1988). Processes in language production. In F. J. Nieuwmeyer (Ed.), Linguistics: The Cambridge survey. Vol. III. Biological and psychological aspects of language (pp. 69—96). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Garrod, S., & Anderson, A. (1987). Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination. Cognition, 27, 181—218.

Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J. (2004). Why is conversation so easy? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 8—11.

Garrod, S., & Sanford, A. J. (1977). Interpreting anaphoric relations: The integration of semantic information while reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 77—90.

Geiselman, R. E., Fisher, R. P., MacKinnon, D. P., & Holland, H. L. (1986). Enhancement of eyewitness memory with the cognitive interview. American Journal of Psychology, 99, 385—401.

Gentner, T. Q., Fenn, K. M., Margoliash, D., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2006). Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds. Nature, 440, 1204—1207.

Gernsbacher, M. A., & Kaschak, M. P. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of language production and comprehension. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 91—114.

Gibson, E., & Pearlmutter, N. J. (1998). Constraints on sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(7), 262—268.

Gibson, J. J. (1979). Ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 306—355.

Glanzer, M., & Cunitz, A. R. (1966). Two storage mechanisms in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 351—360.

Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 558—565.

Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1996). The roles of recognition processes and look-ahead search in time-constrained expert problem solving: Evidence from grand-master-level chess. Psychological Science, 7, 52—55.

Godden, D. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1975). Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: On land and underwater. British Journal of Psychology, 66, 325—331.

Goel, V. (2007). Anatomy of deductive reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 435—441.

Goel, V. (2010). Neural basis of thinking: Laboratory problems versus real-world problems. WIREs Cognitive Science, 1, 613—621.

Golinkoff, R. M., Mervis, C. B., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1994). Early object labels: The case for a developmental lexical principles framework. Journal of Child Language, 21, 125—155.

Gosche, K. M., Mortimer, J. A., Smith, C. D., Markesbery, W. R., & Snowdon, D. A. (2002). An automated technique for measuring hippocampal volumes from MR imaging studies. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 22, 1686—1689.

Grant, E. R., & Spivey, M. J. (2003). Eye movements and problem solving: Guiding attention guides thought. Psychological Science, 14(5), 462—466.

Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Griffin, Z. M. (2004). Why look? Reasons for eye movements related to language production. In J. M. Henderson & F. Ferreira (Eds.), The interface of language, vision, and action: Eye movements and the visual world (pp. 213—248). New York: Psychology Press.

Griggs, R. A., & Cox, J. R. (1982). The elusive thematic-materials effect in Wason’s selection task. British Journal of Psychology, 73, 407—420.

Grill-Spector, K. (2008). What has fMRI taught us about object recognition? In S. J. Dickinson, A. Leonardis, B. Schiele, & M. J. Tarr (Eds.), Object categorization: Computer and human vision perspectives (pp. 102—128). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Gross, C. G. (2002). Genealogy of the “grandmother cell.” Neuroscientist, 8, 512—518.

Haider, H., & Frensch, P. A. (1999). Eye movement during skill acquisition: More evidence for the information-reduction hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(1), 172—190.

Halpern, A. R. (1988). Mental scanning in auditory imagery for songs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 434—443.

Hampton, J. A. (1979). Polymorphous concepts in semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 441—461.

Hampton, J. A. (1982). A demonstration of intransitivity in natural categories. Cognition, 12, 151—164.

Hampton, J. A. (1997). Associative and similarity-based processes in categorization decisions. Memory & Cognition, 25, 625—640.

Hanson, V. I. (1990). Recall of order information by deaf signers: Phonetic coding in temporal order recall. Memory & Cognition, 18, 604—610.

Harlow, J. M. (1868/1993). Recovery from the passage of an iron bar through the head. History of Psychiatry, 4, 274—281.

Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

Hashtroudi, S., Johnson, M. K., & Chrosniak, L. D. (1990). Aging and qualitative characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined complex events. Psychology and Aging, 5, 119—126.

Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., & Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41, 301—307.

Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298, 1569—1579.

Healy, A. F. (1974). Separating item from order information in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 644—655.

Hegarty, M. (1992). Mental animation: Inferring movement from static displays of mechanical systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1084—1102.

Hegarty, M. (2004). Dynamic visualizations and learning: Getting to the difficult questions. Learning and Instruction, 14, 343—351.

Heit, E., & Rubinstein, J. (1994). Similarity and property effects in inductive reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 411—422.

Henson, R. (2005). What can functional neuroimaging tell the experimental psychologist? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193—234.

Henson, R. (2006). Forward inference using functional neuroimaging: Dissociations versus associations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 64—69.

Higuchi, T., Murai, G., Kijima, A., Seya, Y., Wagman, J. B., & Imanaka, K. (2011). Athletic experience influences shoulder rotations when running through apertures. Human Movement Science, 30, 534—549.

Hilton, J. L., & von Hippel, W. (1996). Stereotypes. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 237—271.

Hilts, P. J. (1996). Memory’s ghost: The nature of memory and the strange tale of Mr. M. New York: Simon & Schuster.

Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hollich, G. (2000). An emergentist coalition model for word learning: Mapping words to objects is a product of the interaction of multiple cues. In R. M. Golinkoff, K. Hirsh-Pasek, L. Bloom, L. B. Smith, A. L. Woodard, N. Akhtar, et al. (Eds.), Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition (pp. 179—186). New York: Oxford University Press.

Hockett, C. (1960). The origin of speech. Scientific American, 203, 88—111.

Hollich, G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Brand, R. J., Brown, E., Chung, H. L., et al. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65, (3, Serial No. 262).

Holyoak, K. J., & Koh, K. (1987). Surface and structural similarity in analogical transfer. Memory & Cognition, 15, 332—340.

Hommel, B. (1993). The role of attention for the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 55, 208—222.

Hsee, C. K. (2000). Attribute evaluability: Its implications for joint—separate evaluation reversals and beyond. In D. Kahneman & A. Tversky (Eds.), Choices, values, and frames (pp. 543—563). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Hubbard, T. L. (2010). Auditory imagery: Empirical findings. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 302—329.

Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1959). Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat’s striate cortex. Journal of Physiology, 148, 574—591.

Huettig, F., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2010). Listening to yourself is like listening to others: External, but not internal, verbal self-monitoring is based on speech perception. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(3), 347—374.

Hulme, C., Thompson, N., Muir, C., & Lawrence, A. (1984). Speech rate and the development of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 241—253.

Humphrey, K. H., & Bock, J. K. (2005). Notional number agreement in English. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(4), 689—695.

Isarida, T., Isarida, T. K., & Sakai, T. (2012). Effects of study time and meaningfulness on environmental context-dependent recognition. Memory & Cognition, 40, 1225—1235.

Iverson, P., Kuhl, P. K., Akahane-Yamada, R., Diesch, E., Tohkura, Y., Kettermann, A., et al. (2003). A perceptual interference account of acquisition difficulties for non-native phonemes. Cognition, 87, B47—B57.

Jackendoff, R. (1994). Patterns in the mind: Language and human nature. New York: Basic Books.

Jackendoff, R. (2010). Meaning and the lexicon: The parallel architecture, 1975—2010. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Jacoby, L. L., Woloshyn, V., & Kelley, C. (1989). Being famous without being recognized: Unconscious influences of memory produced by dividing attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 115—125.

James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Henry Holt.

Jeannerod, M. (1995). Mental imagery in the motor cortex. Neuropsychologia, 33, 1419—1432.

Jenkins, J. B., & Dallenbach, K. M. (1924). Oblivescence during sleep and waking. American Journal of Psychology, 35, 605—612.

Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2001). Mental models and deduction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(10), 434—442.

Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2006). How we reason. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Steedman, M. J. (1978). The psychology of syllogisms. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 64—99.

Jonides, J., Lewis, R. L., Nee, D. E., Lustig, C. A., Berman, M. G., & Moore, K. S. (2008). The mind and brain of short-term memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 193—224.

Jung-Beeman, M., Bowden, E. M., Haberman, J., Frymiare, J. L., Armabel-Lui, S., Greenblatt, R., et al. (2004). Neural activity when people solve verbal problems with insight. PLoS Biology, 2, 500—510.

Kahana, M. J., & Loftus, G. (1999). Response time versus accuracy in human memory. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of cognition (pp. 323—384). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.

Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80, 237—251.

Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263—292.

Kaplan, C. A., & Simon, H. A. (1990). In search of insight. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 374—419.

Karpicke, J. D., & Blunt, J. R. (2011). Retrieval practice produces more learning than elaborative studying with concept mapping. Science, 331, 772—775.

Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Kelly, M. H., Bock, J. K., & Keil, F. C. (1986). Prototypicality in a linguistic context: Effects on sentence structure. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 59—74.

Keppel, G., & Underwood, B. J. (1962). Proactive inhibition in short-term retention of single items. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1, 153—161.

Kershaw, T. C., & Ohlsson, S. (2004). Multiple causes of difficulty in insight: The case of the nine-dot problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 3—13.

Kinsbourne, M., & George, J. (1974). The mechanism of the word-frequency effect on recognition memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 63—69.

Klatzky, R. L., McCloskey, B., Doherty, S., Pellegrino, J., & Smith, T. (1987). Knowledge about hand movements and knowledge about objects. Journal of Motor Behavior, 19, 187—213.

Klatzky, R. L., Pellegrino, J., McCloskey, B., & Doherty, S. (1989). Can you squeeze a tomato? The role of motor representations in semantic sensibility judgments. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 56—77.

Klauer, K. C., Stahl, C., & Erdfelder, E. (2007). The abstract selection task: New data and an almost comprehensive model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 680—703.

Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., Haider, H., & Rhenius, D. (1999). Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1534—1555.

Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., & Raney, E. G. (2001). An eye movement study of insight problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 29, 1000—1009.

Köhler, W. (1959). Gestalt psychology today. American Psychologist, 14, 727—734.

Kosslyn, S. M. (1973). Scanning visual images. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 90—94.

Kosslyn, S. M., Alpert, N. M., Thompson, W. L., Maljkovic, V., Weise, S. B., Chabris, C. F., et al. (1993). Visual mental imagery activates topographically organized visual cortex: PET investigations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 263—287.

Kosslyn, S. M., Ball, T. M., & Reiser, B. J. (1978). Visual images preserve metric spatial information: Evidence from studies of image scanning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 47—60.

Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2006). Mental imagery and the human brain. In Q. Jin, M. R. Rosenzweig, G. d’Ydewalle, H. Zhang, H.-C. Chen, & K. Zhang (Eds.), Progress in psychological science around the world: Neural, cognitive, and developmental issues (Vol. 1, pp. 195—209). New York: Psychology Press.

Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., Kim, I. J., & Alpert, N. M. (1995). Topographical representations of mental images in primary visual cortex. Nature, 378, 496—498.

Kounios, J., & Beeman, M. (2009). The aha! moment: The cognitive neuroscience of insight. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 210—216.

Kuhl, P. K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A., Deguchi, T., Kiritani, S., & Iverson, P. (2006). Infants show facilitation for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science, 9, 13—21.

Kvavilashvili, L., Kornbrot, D., Mash, V., Cockburn, J., & Milne, A. (2009). Differential effects of age on prospective and retrospective memory tasks in young, young-old, and old-old adults. Memory, 17, 180—196.

LaBerge, D. (1983). Spatial extent of attention to letters and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 371—379.

LaBerge, D., & Brown, V. (1986). Variations in size of the visual field in which targets are presented: An attentional range effect. Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 188—200.

LaBerge, D., Carlson, R. L., Williams, J. K., & Bunney, B. G. (1997). Shifting attention in visual space: Tests of moving-spotlight models versus an activity-distribution model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1380—1392.

Larkin, J. H., & Simon, H. A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words. Cognitive Science, 11, 65—100.

Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior (pp. 112—136). New York: Wiley.

Lassaline, M. E. (1996). Structural alignment in induction and similarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 754—770.

Lemonick, M. D. (2016). The perpetual now: A story of amnesia, memory, and love. New York: Doubleday.

LePort, A. K. R., Mattfeld, A. T., Dickinson-Anson, H., Fallon, J. H., Stark, C. E. L., Kruggel, F., et al. (2012). Behavioral and neuroanatomical investigation of highly superior autobiographical memory (HSAM). Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 98, 78—92.

Lesgold, A., Rubinson, H., Feltovich, P., Glaser, R., Klopfer, D., & Wang, Y. (1988). Expertise in a complex skill: Diagnosing X-ray pictures. In M. T. H. Chi, R. Glaser, & M. J. Farr (Eds.), The nature of expertise (pp. 311—342). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Levelt, W. J. M. (1983). Monitoring and self-repair in speech. Cognition, 14, 41—104.

Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Levelt, W. J. M. (2012). A history of psycholinguistics: The pre-Chomskyan era. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Eimas, P., Lisker, L., & Bastian, J. (1961). An effect of learning on speech perception: The discrimination of durations of silence with and without phonemic significance. Audiology and Speech-Language Pathology, 53, 175—195.

Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Hoffman, H. S., & Griffith, B. C. (1957). The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 358—368.

Liberman, P. (1984). The biology and evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Libet, B. (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 529—566.

Lin, E. L., & Murphy, G. (1997). Effects of background knowledge on object categorization and part detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(4), 1153—1169.

Lindsay, D. S. (1990). Misleading suggestions can impair eyewitnesses’ ability to remember event details. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 1077—1083.

Loewenstein, G., Rick, S., & Cohen, J. D. (2008). Neuroeconomics. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 647—672.

Loftus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48, 518—537.

Loftus, E. F. (2005). Planting misinformation in the human mind: A 30-year investigation of the malleability of memory. Learning & Memory, 12, 361—366.

Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of the automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 585—589.

Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492—527.

Logan, G. D. (1990). Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms? Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1—35.

Logan, G. D. (1992). Shapes of reaction-time distributions and shapes of learning curves: A test of the instance theory of automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 883—914.

Lu, Z.-L., Neuse, J., Madigan, S., & Dosher, B. A. (2005). Fast decay of iconic memory in observers with mild cognitive impairment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 1797—1802.

Lu, Z.-L., Williamson, S. J., & Kaufman, L. (1992a). Human auditory primary and association cortex have differing lifetimes for activation traces. Brain Research, 572, 236—241.

Lu, Z.-L., Williamson, S. J., & Kaufman, L. (1992b). Behavioral lifetime of human auditory sensory memory predicted by physiological measures. Science, 258, 1668—1670.

Luchins, A. S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung. Psychological Monographs, 54(6), 95.

Luo, J., & Knoblich, G. (2007). Studying insight problem solving with neuroscientific methods. Methods, 42, 77—86.

MacGregor, J. N., Ormerod, T. C., & Chronicle, E. P. (2001). Information processing and insight: A process model of performance on the nine dot and related problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 176—201.

Mack, M. L., Preston, A. R., & Love, B. C. (2013). Decoding the brain’s algorithm for categorization from its neural implementation. Current Biology, 23, 2023—2027.

Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Milne, A. B. (1995). The dissection of selection in person perception: Inhibitory processes in social stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 397—407.

Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (1994). Stereotypes as energy-saving devices: A peek inside the cognitive toolbox. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 37—47.

Maeder, P. P., Meuli, R. A., Adriani, M., Bellmann, A., Fornari, E., Thiran, J.-P., et al. (2001). Distinct pathways involved in sounds recognition and localization: A human fMRI study. NeuroImage, 14, 802—816.

Maguire, E. A., Valentine, E. R., Wilding, J. M., & Kapur, N. (2003). Routes to remembering: The brains behind superior memory. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 90—95.

Mahler, J., Jusczyk, P., Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J., & Amiel-Tison, C. (1988). A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29, 143—178.

Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2009). Concepts and categories: A cognitive neuropsychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 27—51.

Maier, N. R. F. (1930). Reasoning in humans: I. On direction. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 10, 115—143.

Maier, N. R. F. (1931). Reasoning in humans: II. The solution of a problem and its appearance in consciousness. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 12, 181—194.

Malcolm, G. L., Nuthmann, A., & Schyns, P. G. (2014). Beyond gist: Strategic and incremental information accumulation for scene categorization. Psychological Science, 25, 1087—1097.

Malek, E. A., & Wagman, J. B. (2008). Kinetic potential influences visual and remote haptic perception of affordances for standing on an inclined surface. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1813—1826.

Malt, B. C. (1989). An on-line investigation of prototype and exemplar strategies in classification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(4), 539—555.

Markman, A. B., & Wisniewski, E. J. (1997). Similar and different: The differentiation of basic-level categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 54—70.

Markman, E. M. (1989). Categorization and naming in children: Problems of induction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Martin, K. A., Moritz, S. E., & Hall, C. R. (1999). Imagery use in sport: A literature review and applied model. Sport Psychologist, 13, 245—268.

Mayberry, E. J., Sage, K., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2011). At the edge of semantic space: The breakdown of coherent concepts in semantic dementia is constrained by typicality and severity but not modality. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 2240—2251.

McBeath, M. K., Shaffer, D. M., & Kaiser, M. K. (1995). How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls. Science, 268, 569—573.

McBride, D. M., Beckner, J. K., & Abney, D. H. (2011). Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory performance. Memory & Cognition, 39, 1222—1231.

McClelland, J. L. (1999). Cognitive modeling, connectionist. In R. A. Wilson & F. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences (pp. 137—139). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375—407.

McCloskey, M. E., & Glucksberg, S. (1978). Natural categories: Well defined or fuzzy sets? Memory & Cognition, 6, 462—472.

McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (1986). Bizarre imagery as an effective aid: The importance of distinctiveness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 54—65.

McDaniel, M. A., Einstein, G. O., DeLosh, E. L., May, C. P., & Brady, P. (1995). The bizarreness effect: It’s not surprising, it’s complex. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 422—435.

McDaniel, M. A., LaMontagne, P., Beck, S. M., Scullin, M. K., & Braver, T. S. (2013). Dissociable neural routes to successful prospective memory. Psychological Science, 24, 1791—1800.

McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory: A century of consolidation. Science, 287, 248—251.

McIntosh, R. D., & Lashley, G. (2008). Matching boxes: Familiar size influences action programming. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2441—2444.

McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1992). Spreading activation versus compound cue accounts of priming: Mediated priming revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1155—1172.

Medin, D. L., Lynch, E. B., Coley, J. D., & Atran, S. (1997). Categorization and reasoning among tree experts: Do all roads lead to Rome? Cognitive Psychology, 32, 49—96.

Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. (1978). A context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85, 207—238.

Meier, B., & Graf, P. (2000). Transfer appropriate processing for prospective memory tests. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, S11—S27.

Meints, K., Plunkett, K., & Harris, P. L. (1999). When does an ostrich become a bird? The role of typicality in early word comprehension. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1072—1078.

Melton, A. W. (1970). The situation with respect to the spacing of repetitions and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 596—606.

Memon, A., Meissner, C. A., & Fraser, J. (2010). The cognitive interview: A meta-analytic review and study space analysis of the past 25 years. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 16, 340—372.

Mervis, C. B., Catlin, J., & Rosch, E. (1976). Relationships among goodness-of-example, category norms, and word frequency. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 7, 283—294.

Metcalfe, J. A., & Wiebe, D. (1987). Intuition in insight and noninsight problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 15, 238—246.

Meyer, D. E., Osman, A. M., Irwin, D. E., & Yantis, S. (1988). Modern mental chronometry. Biological Psychology, 26, 3—67.

Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227—234.

Miller, G. A. (1951). Language and communication. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81—97.

Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two visual streams re-viewed. Neuropsychologia, 46, 774—785.

Mintzer, M. Z., & Snodgrass, J. G. (1999). The picture superiority effect: Support for the distinctiveness model. American Journal of Psychology, 112, 113—146.

Mitchell, T. M., Shinkareva, S. V., Carlson, A., Chang, K.-M., Malave, V. L., Mason, R. A., et al. (2008). Predicting human brain activity associated with the meanings of nouns. Science, 320, 1191—1195.

Monsell, S. (1991). The nature and locus of word frequency effects in reading. In D. Besner & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 148—197). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 56—60.

Moreno, F. J., Reina, R., Luis, V., & Sabido, R. (2002). Visual search strategies in experienced and inexperienced gymnastic coaches. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 95, 901—902.

Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 519—533.

Moscovitch, M., Chein, J. M., Talmi, D., & Cohn, M. (2007). Learning and memory. In B. J. Baars & N. M. Gage (Eds.), Cognition, brain, and consciousness: Introduction to cognitive neuroscience. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Moser, E. I., Kropff, E., & Moser, M.-B. (2008). Place cells, grid cells, and the brain’s spatial representation system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31, 69—89.

Moulton, S. T., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2009). Imagining predictions: Mental imagery and mental emulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 1273—1280.

Mu, Y., & Gage, F. H. (2011). Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and its role in Alzheimer’s disease. Molecular Neurodegeneration, 6, 1—9.

Mulligan, N. M. (2012). Differentiating between conceptual implicit and explicit memory: A crossed double dissociation between category-exemplar production and category-cued recall. Psychological Science, 23, 404—406.

Murphy, G. L. (2002). The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Murphy, G. L., & Brownell, H. H. (1985). Category differentiation in object recognition: Typicality constraints on the basic category advantage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 70—84.

Murphy, G. L., Hampton, J. A., & Milovanovic, G. S. (2012). Semantic memory redux: An experimental test of hierarchical category representation. Journal of Memory and Language, 67, 521—539.

Murphy, G. L., & Lassaline, M. E. (1997). Hierarchical structure in concepts and the basic level of categorization. In K. Lamberts & D. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, concepts, and categories (pp. 93—131). London: Psychology Press.

Murphy, G. L., & Medin, D. L. (1985). The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review, 92, 289—316.

Murphy, G. L., & Ross, B. H. (2005). Two faces of typicality in category-based induction. Cognition, 95, 175—200.

Murphy, G. L., & Wright, J. C. (1984). Changes in conceptual structure with expertise: Differences between real-world experts and novices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10, 144—155.

Mynatt, C. R., Doherty, M. E., & Dragan, W. (1993). Information relevance, working memory, and the consideration of alternatives. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46A, 759—778.

Nairne, J. S., & Neath, I. (2013). Sensory and working memory. In A. F. Healy & R. W. Proctor (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of psychology (2nd ed.), Vol. 4: Experimental psychology (pp. 419—445). New York: Wiley.

Nairne, J. S., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2008). Adaptive memory: Remembering with a stone-age brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 239—243.

Nairne, J. S., Van Arsdall, J. E., Pandeirada, J. N. S., Cogdill, M., & LeBreton, J. M. (2013). Adaptive memory: The mnemonic value of animacy. Psychological Science, 24, 2099—2105.

Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Ayres, T. J. (1986). Digit span, reading rate, and linguistic relativity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 739—751.

Neath, I., & Knoedler, A. J. (1994). Distinctiveness and serial position effects in recognition and sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 776—795.

Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Nelson, D. L., Kitto, K., Galea, D., McEvoy, C. L., & Bruza, P. D. (2013). How activation, entanglement, and searching a semantic network contribute to event memory. Memory & Cognition, 41, 797—819.

Newell, A., & Simon, H. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Newsome, W. T., Britten, K. H., & Movshon, J. A. (1989). Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision. Nature, 341, 52—54.

Nicoletti, R., & Umiltá, C. (1989). Splitting visual space with attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 164—169.

Norman, D., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. Davidson, R. G. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 1—18). New York: Plenum Press.

Noveck, I. A., & Reboul, A. (2008). Experimental pragmatics: A Gricean turn in the study of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 425—431.

Novick, L. R. (1988). Analogical transfer, problem similarity, and expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(3), 510—520.

Nozari, N., Dell, G. S., & Schwartz, M. F. (2011). Is comprehension necessary for error detection? A conflict-based account of monitoring in speech production. Cognitive Psychology, 63, 1—33.

Oaksford, M. R., & Chater, N. (1994). A rational analysis of the selection task as optimal data selection. Psychological Review, 101, 608—631.

Ohlsson, S. (1992). Information-processing models of insight and related phenomena. In M. T. Keane & K. J. Gilhooly (Eds.), Advances in the psychology of thinking (Vol. 1, pp. 1—44). New York: Harvester/Wheatsheaf.

O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK.

Oyama, T., Simizu, M., & Tozawa, J. (1999). Effects of similarity on apparent motion and perceptual grouping. Perception, 28, 739—748.

Padgitt, A. J., & Hund, A. M. (2012). How good are these directions? Determining direction quality and wayfinding efficiency. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 32, 164—172.

Paivio, A. (1975). Coding distinctions and repetition effects in memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 9, pp. 179—215). New York: Academic Press.

Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press.

Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45, 255—287.

Paivio, A. (1995). Imagery and memory. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 977—986). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Paivio, A., & Csapo, K. (1973). Picture superiority in free recall: Imagery or dual coding? Cognitive Psychology, 5, 176—206.

Paivio, A., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Imagery and association value in paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 35—39.

Parker, E. S., Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (2006). A case of unusual autobiographical remembering. Neurocase, 12, 35—49.

Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 8, 976—987.

Pavlenko, A. (1999). New approaches to concepts in bilingual memory. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 2, 209—230.

Payne, D. G., Elie, C. J., Blackwell, J. M., & Neuschatz, J. S. (1996). Memory illusions: Recalling, recognizing, and recollecting events that never occurred. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 261—285.

Pepperberg, I. M. (2009). The Alex studies: Cognitive and communicative abilities of grey parrots. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Peterson, L. R., & Johnson, S. T. (1971). Some effects of minimizing articulation on short-term retention. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 346—354.

Peterson, L. R., & Peterson, M. J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193—198.

Peterson, R. R., & Savoy, P. (1998). Lexical selection and phonological encoding during language production: Evidence for cascaded processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 539—557.

Pilley, J. W., & Reid, A. K. (2011). Border collie comprehends object names as verbal referents. Behavioural Processes, 86, 184—195.

Pinker, S., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1978). The representation and manipulation of three-dimensional space in mental images. Journal of Mental Imagery, 2, 69—83.

Pobric, G., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010). Category-specific versus category-general semantic impairment induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Current Biology, 20, 964—968.

Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 59—63.

Polya, G. (1957). How to solve it. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Pomerantz, J. R., & Portillo, M. C. (2012). Grouping and emergent features in vision: Toward a theory of basic Gestalts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1331—1349.

Pongrácz, P., Molnár, C., & Miklósi, Á. (2006). Acoustic parameters of dog barks carry emotional information for humans. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 100, 228—240.

Posner, M. I. (2005). Timing in the brain: Mental chronometry as a tool in neuroscience. PLoS Biology, 3, 204—206.

Premack, A. J., & Premack, D. (1972). Teaching language to an ape. Scientific American, 227, 92—99.

Premack, D. (1988). Minds with and without language. In L. Weiskrantz (Ed.), Thought without language (pp. 46—65). New York: Oxford University Press.

Pretz, J. E., Naples, A. J., & Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Recognizing, defining, and representing problems. In J. E. Davidson & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of problem solving (pp. 3—30). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in perceiving distance. Psychological Science, 14, 106—112.

Pruden, S. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hennon, E. A. (2006). The birth of words: Ten-month-olds learn words through perceptual salience. Child Development, 77, 266—280.

Pulvermüller, F. (2010a). Brain embodiment of syntax and grammar: Discrete combinatorial mechanisms spelt out in neural circuits. Brain & Language, 112, 167—179.

Pulvermüller, F. (2010b). Brain language research: Where is the progress? Biolinguistics, 4, 255—288.

Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1973). What the mind’s eye tells the mind’s brain: A critique of mental imagery. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 1—24.

Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1981). The imagery debate: Analogue media versus tacit knowledge. Psychological Review, 87, 16—45.

Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2002). Mental imagery: In search of a theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 157—238.

Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2003). Return of the mental image: Are there pictures in the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 113—118.

Quinn, J. G., & McConnell, J. (1996). Irrelevant pictures in working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 200—215.

Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., & Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature, 23, 1102—1107.

Ramos, D., & Ades, C. (2012). Two-item sentence comprehension by a dog (Canis familiaris). PLoS One, 7(2): e29689.

Rauschecker, J. P., & Tian, B. (2000). Mechanisms and streams for processing of “what” and “where” in auditory cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97, 11800—11806.

Rayner, K., Carlson, M., & Frazier, L. (1983). The interaction of syntax and semantics during sentence processing: Eye movements in the analysis of semantically biased sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 358—374.

Rayner, K., & Duffy, S. A. (1986). Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Memory & Cognition, 14, 191—201.

Reicher, G. M. (1969). Perceptual recognition as a function of meaningfulness of stimulus material. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 275—280.

Reingold, E. M., Charness, N., Pomplun, M., & Stampe, D. M. (2001). Visual span in expert chess players: Evidence from eye movements. Psychological Science, 12(1), 48—55.

Revlis, R. (1975). Syllogistic reasoning: Logical decisions from a complex data base. In R. J. Falmagne (Ed.), Reasoning: Representation and process. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Richardson, D. C., & Dale, R. (2005). Looking to understand: The coupling between speakers’ and listeners’ eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension. Cognitive Science, 29, 1045—1060.

Richardson, D. C., & Spivey, M. J. (2000). Representation, space and Hollywood Squares: Looking at things that aren’t there anymore. Cognition, 76, 269—295.

Richmond, J., & Nelson, C. A. (2007). Accounting for change in declarative memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Developmental Review, 27, 349—373.

Rick, S. (2011). Losses, gains, and brains: Neuroeconomics can help to answer open questions about loss aversion. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21, 453—463.

Rips, L. J. (1975). Inductive judgments about natural categories. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 665—681.

Rips, L. J. (1989). Similarity, typicality, and categorization. In S. Vosniadou & A. Ortony (Eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning (pp. 21—59). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Rips, L. J. (1994). The psychology of proof. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Rips, L. J., Shoben, E. J., & Smith, E. E. (1973). Semantic distance and the verification of semantic relations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 1—20.

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131—141.

Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169—192.

Roberts, M. J. (2005). Expanding the universe of categorical syllogisms: A challenge for reasoning researchers. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 560—580.

Roberts, M. J., Newstead, S. E., & Griggs, R. S. (2001). Quantifier interpretation and syllogistic reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 7, 173—204.

Roberts, M. J., & Sykes, E. D. A. (2005). Categorical reasoning from multiple premises. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 333—376.

Roediger, H. L. III. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without remembering. American Psychologist, 45, 1043—1056.

Roediger, H. L. III, & Karpicke, J. D. (2006a). Test enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17, 249—255.

Roediger, H. L. III, & Karpicke, J. D. (2006b). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 181—210.

Roediger, H. L. III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803—814.

Roediger, H. L. III, & Pyc, M. A. (2012). Inexpensive techniques to improve education: Applying cognitive psychology to enhance educational practice. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 242—248.

Rogers, T., & McClelland, J. (2004). Semantic cognition: A parallel distributed processing approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Rorden, C., & Karnath, H. O. (2004). Using human brain lesions to infer function: A relic from a past era in the fMRI age? Nature Reviews, 5, 813—819.

Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27—48). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblance: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573—605.

Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W., Johnson, D., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382—439.

Rosenbaum, D. A. (2012). The tiger on your tail: Choosing between temporally extended behaviors. Psychological Science, 23, 855—860.

Rosenbaum, D. A., Brach, M., & Semenov, A. (2011). Behavioral ecology meets motor Journal of Motor Behavior, 43, 131—136.

Ross, B. H. (1984). Remindings and their effects in learning a cognitive skill. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 371—416.

Ross, B. H. (1989). Distinguishing types of superficial similarities: Different effects on the access and use of earlier problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 456—468.

Ross, B. H., & Kilbane, M. C. (1997). Effects of principle explanation and superficial similarity on analogical mapping in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 427—440.

Rumelhart, D. E., & Abrahamson, A. A. (1973). Toward a theory of analogical reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 1—28.

Rumelhart, D. E., & Norman, D. A. (1988). Representation in memory. In R. C. Atkinson, R. J. Herrnstein, G. Lindzey, & R. D. Luce (Eds.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology (2nd ed., pp. 511—587). New York: Wiley.

Rustichini, A. (2009). Neuroeconomics: What have we found, and what should we search for. Current Opinion on Neurobiology, 19, 672—677.

Sach, A. T., Kohler, A., Bestmann, S., Linden, D. E. J., Dechent, O., Goebel, R., et al. (2007). Imaging the brain activity changes underlying impaired visuospatial judgments: Simultaneous fMRI, TMS, and behavioral studies. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2841—2852.

Sacks, O. (1990). The man who mistook his wife for a hat. New York: Harper Perennial.

Salomon, M. M., Magliano, J. P., & Radvansky, G. A. (2013). Verb aspect and problem solving. Cognition, 128, 134—139.

Sanfey, A. G., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2006). Neuroeconomics: Cross-currents in research on decision-making. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 108—116.

Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1993). Kanzi: A most improbable ape. Tokyo, Japan: NHK Publishing.

Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Fields, W. M., & Spircu, T. (2004). The emergence of knapping and vocal expression embedded in a Pan/Homo culture. Biology and Philosophy, 19, 541—575.

Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Murphy, J., Sevcik, R. A., Brakke, K. E., Williams, S. L., & Rumbaugh, D. M. (1993). Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 233, 1—252.

Schacter, D. L. (2002). The seven sins of memory: How the mind forgets and remembers. New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

Schiller, P. H. (1966). Developmental study of color-word interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 105—108.

Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1—66.

Schriefers, H., Meyer, A. S., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1990). Exploring the time course of lexical access in speech production: Picture-word interference studies. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 86—102.

Schurger, A., Sitt, J. D., & Dehaene, S. (2012). An accumulator model for spontaneous neural activity prior to self-initiated movement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, E2904—E2913.

Schwarzkopf, D. S., Song, C., & Rees, G. (2011). The surface area of human V1 predicts the subjective experience of object size. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 28—30.

Scott, C. L., Harris, R. J., & Rothe, A. R. (2001). Embodied cognition through improvisation improves memory for a dramatic monologue. Discourse Processes, 31, 293—305.

Segaert, K., Menenti, L., Weber, K., Petersson, K. M., & Hagoort, P. (2012). Shared syntax in language production and language comprehension: An fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 1662—1670.

Sellen, A. J., Louie, G., Harris, G. E., & Wilkins, A. J. (1997). What brings attention to mind? An in situ study of prospective memory. Memory, 5, 483—507.

Shaffer, D. M., Krauchunas, S. M., Eddy, M., & McBeath, M. K. (2004). How dogs navigate to catch Frisbees. Psychological Science, 15, 437—441.

Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701—703.

Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127—190.

Simon, H. A. (1974). How big is a chunk? Science, 183, 482—488.

Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174—176.

Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300—304.

Simon, J. R., & Wolf, J. R. (1963). Choice reaction time as a function of angular stimulus-response correspondence and age. Ergonomics, 6, 99—105.

Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059—1074.

Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (1998). Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 644—649.

Simpson, G. B., & Burgess, C. (1985). Activation and selection processes in the recognition of ambiguous words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(1), 28—39.

Sinclair, R. J., & Burton, H. (1996). Discrimination of vibrotactile frequencies in a delayed pair comparison task. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 680—692.

Singer, M. (1994). Discourse inference processes. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 479—515). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Sinha, I., & Smith, M. F. (2000). Consumers’ perceptions of promotional framing of price. Psychology & Marketing, 17, 257—275.

Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Sloman, S. A. (2005). Causal models. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Slotnick, S. D., Thompson, W. L., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2012). Visual memory and visual mental imagery recruit common control and sensory regions of the brain. Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 14—20.

Sloutsky, V. M., Kloos, H., & Fisher, A. V. (2007). When looks are everything: Appearance similarity versus kind information in early induction. Psychological Science, 18, 179—185.

Smith, E. E., & Osherson, D. N. (1984). Conceptual combination with prototype concepts. Cognitive Science, 8, 357—361.

Smith, E. E., Osherson, D. N., Rips, L. J., & Keane, M. (1988). Combining prototypes: A selective modification model. Cognitive Science, 12, 485—527.

Smith, E. E., Rips, L. J., & Shoben, E. J. (1974). Semantic memory and psychological semantics. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 1—45). New York: Academic Press.

Smith, J. D., & Minda, J. P. (2000). Thirty categorization results in search of a model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 3—27.

Smith, S. M., & Vela, E. (2001). Environmental context-dependent memory: A review and meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 203—220.

Snodgrass, J. G., & McClure, P. (1975). Storage and retrieval properties of dual codes for pictures and words in recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1, 521—529.

Snow, C. E. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language, 4, 1—22.

Sperling, G. (1960). The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs, 74(498), 1—29.

Squire, L. R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 82, 171—177.

Squire, L. R. (2009). The legacy of patient H. M. for neuroscience. Neuron, 61, 6—9.

Stark, C. E. L., Okado, Y., & Loftus, E. F. (2010). Imaging the reconstruction of true and false memories using sensory reactivation and the misinformation paradigm. Learning and Memory, 17, 485—488.

Steblay, N. M., Dysart, J., Fulero, S., & Lindsay, R. C. L. (2001). Eyewitness accuracy rates in sequential and simultaneous lineup presentations: A meta-analytic comparison. Law and Human Behavior, 25, 459—474.

Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Thinking styles. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Stickgold, R., Hobson, J. A., Fosse, H. R., & Fosse, M. (2001). Sleep, learning, and dreams: Off-line memory processing. Science, 294, 1052—1057.

Strayer, D. L., & Johnston, W. A. (2001). Driven to distraction: Dual-task studies of simulated driving and conversing on a cellular telephone. Psychological Science, 12, 462—466.

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interferences in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643—662.

Swinney, D. (1979). Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)-consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 645—659.

Talarico, J. M., & Rubin, D. C. (2003). Confidence, not consistency, characterizes flashbulb memories. Psychological Science, 14, 455—461.

Tanaka, J. W., & Taylor, M. (1991). Object categories and expertise: Is the basic level in the eye of the beholder? Cognitive Psychology, 23, 457—482.

Taraban, R., & McClelland, J. L. (1988). Constituent attachment and thematic role assignment in sentence processing: Influences of content-based expectations. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 597—632.

Terrace, H. S., Pettito, L. A., Sanders, R. J., & Bever, T. G. (1979). Can apes create a sentence? Science, 206, 891—902.

Thaler, R. H. (1985). Mental accounting matters. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12, 183—206.

Thomas, A. K., & Loftus, E. F. (2002). Creating bizarre false memories through imagination. Memory & Cognition, 30, 423—431.

Thomas, L. E. (2013). Spatial working memory is necessary for actions to guide thought. Journal of Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(6), 1974—1981.

Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of semantic memory: Inferring “how” from “where.” Neuropsychologia, 41, 280—292.

Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal intelligence: Experimental studies. New York: Macmillan.

Tracy, R. J., & Barker, C. H. (1993). A comparison of visual versus auditory imagery in predicting word recall. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 13, 147—161.

Treisman, A. M. (1960). Contextual cues in selective listening. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 242—248.

Treisman, A. M. (1961). Attention and speech (Unpublished dissertation). Oxford University, Oxford, UK.

Treisman, A. M. (1964). Monitoring and storage of irrelevant messages in selective attention. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3, 449—459.

Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97—136.

Treisman, A. M., Sykes, M., & Gelade, G. (1977). Selective attention and stimulus integration. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention and performance IV (pp. 333—361). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Turvey, M. T., Brick, P., & Osborn, J. (1970). Proactive interference in short-term memory as a function of prior-item retention interval. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 142—147.

Tversky, A. (1972). Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. Psychological Review, 79, 281—299.

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207—232.

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453—458.

Tweney, R. D., Doherty, M. E., Worner, W. J., Pliske, D. B., Mynatt, C. R., Gross, K. A., et al. (1980). Strategies of rule discovery in an inference task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 109—123.

Ungerleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. V. (1994). “What” and “where” in the human brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 4, 157—165.

VanDenBroek, P., Lorch, R. F., Linderholm, T., & Gustafson, M. (2001). The effects of readers’ goals on inference generation and memory for texts. Memory & Cognition, 29(8), 1081—1087.

Vigliocco, G., Antonini, T., & Garrett, M. F. (1997). Grammatical gender is on the tip of Italian tongues. Psychological Science, 8, 314—317.

Vigliocco, G., & Hartsuiker, R. (2002). The interplay of meaning, sound, and syntax in sentence production. Psychological Bulletin, 128(3), 442—472.

von Frisch, K. (1967). The dance language and orientation of bees. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

von Helmholtz, H. L. F. (1850/1853). Uber die methoden, kleinste Zeittheile zu messen, und ihre Anwendung fur physiologische Zwecked. Original work translated in Philosophical Magazine, 1853, (6 Section 4), 313—325.

Wagemans, J., Elder, J. H., Kubovy, M., Palmer, S. E., Peterson, M. A., Singh, M., et al. (2012). A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: I. Perceptual grouping and figure-ground organization. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1172—1217.

Wagman, J. B., & Hajnal, A. (2014). Task specificity and anatomical independence in perception of properties by means of a wielded object. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 2372—2391.

Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. New York: Harcourt, Brace.

Warren, R. M. (1970). Perceptual restoration of missing speech sounds. Science, 167, 392—393.

Warrington, E. K. (1975). The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 635—657.

Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1968). A new method for testing long-term retention with special reference to amnesic patients. Nature, 217, 972—974.

Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1970). Amnesic syndrome: Consolidation or retrieval. Nature, 228, 628—630.

Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1974). The effect of prior learning on subsequent retention in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 12, 419—428.

Wason, P. C. (1960). On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 129—140.

Wason, P. C. (1968). Reasoning about a rule. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 273—281.

Wason, P. C., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1972). Psychology of reasoning: Structure and content. London: Batsford.

Wason, P. C., & Shapiro, D. A. (1971). Natural and contrived experience in a reasoning problem. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 63—71.

Waugh, N. C., & Norman, D. A. (1965). Primary memory. Psychological Review, 72, 89—104.

Weisberg, R. W. (1988). Problem solving and creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives (pp. 148—176). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Weisberg, R. W., & Alba, J. W. (1981). An examination of the alleged role of “fixation” in the solution of “insight” problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 169—192.

Wells, G. L., & Bradfield, A. L. (1998). “Good, you identified the suspect”: Feedback to eyewitnesses distorts their reports of the witnessing experience. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 360—376.

Wells, G. L., Small, M., Penrod, S., Malpass, R. S., Fulero, S. M., & Brimacombe, C. A. E. (1998). Eyewitness identification procedures: Recommendations for lineups and photo spreads. Law and Human Behavior, 22, 1—39.

Wernicke, C. (1874). The symptom complex of aphasia. Reprinted in English in Proceedings of the Boston Colloquium for the Philosophy of Science, 4, 34—97.

Wertheimer, M. (1959). Productive thinking (Enlarged ed.). New York: Harper and Brothers. (Original work published 1945)

Wetherick, N. E., & Gilhooly, K. J. (1990). Syllogistic reasoning: Effects of premise order. In K. Gilhooly, M. Keane, R. Logie, & G. Erdos (Eds.), Lines of thinking: Reflections on the psychology of thought (Vol. 1, pp. 99—108). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.

Wickens, D. D. (1970). Encoding categories of words: An empirical approach to meaning. Psychological Review, 77, 1—15.

Wiley, J., & Jarosz, A. F. (2012). Working memory capacity, attentional focus, and problem solving. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(4), 258—262.

Wilkins, M. C. (1928). The effect of changed material on ability to do formal syllogistic reasoning. Archives of Psychology, 102, 83.

Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. W. (1991). Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. Attitudes and Social Cognition, 60(2), 181—192.

Witt, J. K., Linkenauger, S. A., & Proffitt, D. R. (2012). Get me out of this slump! Visual illusions improve sports performance. Psychological Science, 23, 397—399.

Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2005). See the ball, hit the ball: Apparent ball size is correlated with batting average. Psychological Science, 16, 937—938.

Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. New York: Macmillan.

Wixted, J. T. (2004). The psychology of neuroscience and forgetting. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 235—269.

Wixted, J. T. (2010). The role of retroactive interference and consolidation in everyday forgetting. In S. Della Sala (Ed.), Forgetting (pp. 285—312). East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.

Wood, N., & Cowan, N. (1995). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: How frequent are attention shifts to one’s name in an irrelevant auditory channel? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 255—260.

Woodworth, R. S., & Sells, S. B. (1935). An atmosphere effect in syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 451—460.

Xu, Y., & Franconeri, S. L. (2015). Capacity for visual features in mental rotation. Psychological Science, 26, 1241—1251.

Zaretskaya, N., Anstis, S., & Bartels, A. (2013). Parietal cortex mediates conscious perception of illusory Gestalt. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 523—531.

Zatorre, R. J., & Halpern, A. R. (2005). Mental concerts: Musical imagery and auditory cortex. Neuron, 47, 9—12.

Zhang, G., & Simon, H. A. (1985). STM capacity for Chinese words and idioms: Chunking and acoustical loop hypotheses. Memory & Cognition, 13, 193—201.

Zu, B., Chen, C., Loftus, E. F., He, Q., Chen, C., Lei, X., et al. (2002). Brief exposure to misinformation can lead to long-term false memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 301—307.

Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 162—185.

Zwaan, R. A., Stanfield, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2002). Do language comprehenders routinely represent the shapes of objects? Psychological Science, 13, 168—171.